Welcome to "Without Conscience" Robert Hare's Web Site devoted to the study of Psychopathy |
WELCOME | AWARDS | PSYCHOPATHY SCALES | PCL-R TRAINING | COMMENTS |
LINKS | PRESENTATIONS | KEY REFERENCES | CONTACT |
#1   #2   #3   HOME |
Over the past few months many newspapers, blogs, and other internet postings have passed on some variation of an incorrect statement by Sherree DeCovny in CFA Magazine:
DeCovny’s statement was picked up by Alexander Eichler, who wrote an article in the May 19, 2012 edition of The Huffington Post titled,
Neither DeCovny nor Eichler contacted me or my colleagues to determine if this information was correct. The first to do so was Dr. John Grohol, who wrote,
Grohol posted my response, along with his views on reporters who do not bother to check their “facts” (psychcentral.com/blog/archives/2012/03/06). Not everyone read Grohol’s web page or my comments. In an article for the May 13, 2012 edition of the New York Times, William Deresiewicz wrote the following under the caption of Capitalists and Other Psychopaths: On May 15, 2012, the Deputy Editor of the Op-Ed page for the New York Times wrote to me:
We exchanged emails and the Deputy Editor arranged for the New York Times to issue a correction, which was as follows:
Correction: May 20, 2012 An opinion essay on May 13 about ethics and capitalism misstated the findings of a 2010 study on psychopathy in corporations. The study found that 4 percent of a sample of 203 corporate professionals met a clinical threshold for being described as psychopaths, not that 10 percent of people who work on Wall Street are clinical psychopaths. In addition, the study, in the journal Behavioral Sciences and the Law, was not based on a representative sample; the authors of the study say that the 4 percent figure cannot be generalized to the larger population of corporate managers and executives.” A Comment on Prevalence As things stand, we do not know the prevalence of psychopathy among those who work on Wall Street. It may be even higher than 10%, on the assumption that psychopathic entrepreneurs and risk-takers tend to gravitate toward financial watering-holes, particularly those that are enormously lucrative and poorly regulated. But, until the research has been conducted, we are left with anecdotal evidence and widespread speculation. Further, in evaluating estimates about the prevalence of psychopathy it is important to note that they typically are based on the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R) or one of its derivatives. PCL-R scores can vary from 0 to 40 and reflect the degree to which the individual matches our conception of the prototypical psychopath. This is in line with recent evidence that (at least at the measurement level) psychopathy is dimensional (i.e., more or less), not categorical (i.e., either or). Dimensionality may pose a problem for diagnosing or categorizing someone as a “psychopath,” a problem shared by other clinical and medical (e.g., anxiety, depression, hypertension, obesity) that often are described and treated as categorical but in fact may be dimensional. But, dimensionality does not preclude the use of “diagnostic” thresholds for making clinical decisions. With respect to psychopathy, a PCL-R threshold of 30 (out of 40) has proven useful for describing persons as psychopathic for research and applied purposes. Those with a “heavy dose” of psychopathic features may pose serious personal, psychological, and financial difficulties for others. However, even those with a somewhat lower dose may present significant problems for those around them, just as those with blood pressure readings below an accepted threshold for hypertension may be at medical risk. Finally, I note that while the study by Babiak, Neumann, & Hare (Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 2010, 28, 174-193) found that close to 4% of the corporate professionals in the study had a PCL-R score of 30 or higher, some 80% had scores from 0 to 3, similar to the pattern of scores found in the general population. The most interesting part of the study was the finding that high scores were positively associated with in-house ratings of charisma/presentation style (creativity, good strategic thinking and communication skills) but negatively associated with ratings of responsibility/performance (being a team player, management skills, and overall accomplishments). Robert D. Hare Download this comment as a PDF
|